Revisiting Phonological Generalizations in Australian Languages*
نویسندگان
چکیده
1.1 Aims and background Australian languages are famous for their near-uniform phonemic inventories. Authors such as Busby (1980); Dixon (1980); Hamilton (1996); Dixon (2002); Butcher (2006), amongst others, have emphasized the similarity of phoneme inventories across the continent, using it as evidence of intensive lexical diffusion and linguistic convergence (Dixon 1997, Dixon 2002:547). Cross-linguistic surveys of Australian languages such as those just mentioned have repeatedly published claims regarding the patterns found in the ‘typical’ Australian language, as well as other phonological generalizations meant to apply more or less equally to all of the approximately 360 languages of the continent. The apparent uniformity of Australian languages also stands out in worldwide typological surveys (Maddieson, 1984; Mielke, 2008; Hunley et al., 2012). Moreover, Australianists have often assumed that uniformity in the phonemic inventory is coupled with uniform phonotactics (Dixon, 2002:547). Otherwise unqualified statements about uniformity in inventory and phonotactics are also easily found in reference grammars of languages in the region (for one example among many, compare Goddard 1985:21, 43, 66, 323). This ‘typical’ inventory is given in Figure 1. This assumption is, in itself, surprising, given that there is no general assumption in phonology that associates uniformity in inventory size or composition with phonotactic generalizations such as syllable structure constraints or segment frequencies. Such phonological uniformity, if real, is surprising and unusual given the country’s phylogenetic diversity. Pace Dixon (2002), there are approximately 28 phylic families (Dixon, 1980; O’Grady et al., 1966), and the largest of these, Pama-Nyungan, has approximately 30 low-level subgroups.1 Lexical differentiation among Pama-Nyungan subgroups is extensive and rates of lexical replacement are high (Bowern & Atkinson, 2012). However, contra Dixon (2001, 2002), rates of borrowing are, for the most part, low (Bowern et al., 2011; Bowern & Atkinson, 2012). The only ways for this large number of languages to be so similar would be through intensive, continent-wide language contact; shallow linguistic relationships; or extreme conservatism in sound change. None of these assumptions is particularly plausible. Bowern et al. (2011) show that levels of language contact are variable and in scale with other languages of the world. Bowern & Atkinson (2012) find high rates of lexical replacement, implying that the linguistic relationships among subgroups of PamaNyungan are not particularly shallow.2 Sound change conservatism has not been investigated but publications such as Alpher (2004); Koch (1996, 1997), and others, provide evidence for sound change in these languages. The claims of uniformity should thus be treated with skepticism. In this paper, we revisit claims of uniformity in Australian languages by testing variation in aspects of the phoneme inventories and phonotactics of a representative sample of languages. We do this by utilizing
منابع مشابه
Phonological representation: Beyond abstract versus episodic
Phonology deals with systematic patterns in the sound structure of languages. Language scientists propose phonological representations with the goal of developing general and predictive understanding of these patterns. We want our understanding of phonology to be general, capturing the implicit knowledge that different speakers of the same language share, as well as similarities and differences...
متن کاملPhonological generalizations in dyslexia: the phonological grammar may not be impaired.
Dyslexia is commonly attributed to a phonological deficit, but whether it effectively compromises the phonological grammar or lower level systems is rarely explored. To address this question, we gauge the sensitivity of dyslexics to grammatical phonological restrictions on spoken onset clusters (e.g., bl in block). Across languages, certain onsets are preferred to others (e.g., blif ≻ bnif ≻ bd...
متن کاملPhonological reduplication in sign language: Rules rule
Productivity-the hallmark of linguistic competence-is typically attributed to algebraic rules that support broad generalizations. Past research on spoken language has documented such generalizations in both adults and infants. But whether algebraic rules form part of the linguistic competence of signers remains unknown. To address this question, here we gauge the generalization afforded by Amer...
متن کاملDirectional Asymmetries in the Morphology and Phonology of Words, with Special Reference to Bantu
This paper is concerned with two types of word-level asymmetries and their interaction: leftright asymmetries and stem-word asymmetries. Two left-right asymmetries are examined from a wide range of languages, one morphological (the predominance of suffixation over prefixation), one phonological (the preference for anticipatory over perseverative phonology). Since phonological processes are ofte...
متن کاملLanguage context elicits native-like stop voicing in early bilinguals' productions in both L1 and L2
The way that bilinguals produce phones in each of their languages provides a window into the nature of the bilingual phonological space. For stop consonants, if early sequential bilinguals, whose languages differ in voice onset time (VOT) distinctions, produce native-like VOTs in each of their languages, it would imply that they have developed separate first and second language phones, that is,...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
عنوان ژورنال:
دوره شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2014